0
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Research Letters |

College Students' Cognitive Rationalizations for Tanning Bed Use: An Exploratory Study FREE

Smita C. Banerjee, PhD; Jennifer L. Hay, PhD; Kathryn Greene, PhD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center, New York, New York (Drs Banerjee and Hay); Department of Communication, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey (Dr Greene).


Arch Dermatol. 2012;148(6):761-762. doi:10.1001/archdermatol.2012.398.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Tanning bed use is a popular behavior among American youth1 despite evidence of increased risk of melanoma.2 Current interventions to reduce tanning bed use3 have not been very successful with resistant tanners, characterized by engagement in tanning bed use for appearance enhancement despite being knowledgeable about harmful effects of their behavior.

This dichotomy between unfavorable attitudinal beliefs (tanning bed use is harmful) and continued tanning bed use can possibly be explained by cognitive dissonance theory.4 According to cognitive dissonance theory, individuals seek consistency among their cognitions. Inconsistent cognitions create psychological discomfort that motivates people to alter their cognitions to restore consistency.4

Prior research demonstrates that individuals often rationalize their existing inconsistent beliefs to decrease dissonance. Therefore, in the case of tanning bed use, people's continued use may be supported by cognitive rationalizations justifying tanning bed use despite awareness of risks.

To identify these rationalizations, we adapted an available measure of cognitive rationalization5 to tanning bed use (original measure explored cognitive rationalizations related to smoking) and conducted a survey with a sample of college students. The aim of this article is to examine the distribution of the item responses to examine how relevant these rationalizations are to our population of interest, ie, current tanning bed users.

After receiving human subjects' approval from the university institutional review board, we surveyed 587 undergraduate students in introductory communication courses at a large university in the northeastern United States. Of the original 587 participants, students older than 25 years were excluded (n = 36) to retain sample homogeneity.

Of the 551 participants included, 218 participants had ever used tanning beds (39.6%). Given that we wanted to examine cognitive rationalizations used by former and current tanners, we utilized the data from these 218 participants to examine the cognitive rationalization scale. Among this group of ever tanners, 87.6% were women (n = 191). The mean (SD) age of participants was 19.98 (1.13) years (age range, 18-24 years), and about 78.4% of participants identified themselves as white, 9.6% Asian, and 7.8% Hispanic/Latino (other groups, <2% each).

We adapted the cognitive rationalization scale developed by Oakes and colleagues5 to tanning bed use. The scale consisted of a common stem for all items, “Tanning bed use can make me ill, but . . . ,” and was measured with 16 Likert-type items, with responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). We used 16 of the 18 items supporting the following 3 a priori factors (we altered the factor names for relevance to tanning bed use) (Table): factor 1, skeptical rationalizations (ie, beliefs indicating tanning bed users do not believe medical evidence about tanning bed use and disease); factor 2, worth-it rationalizations (ie, beliefs indicating tanning bed users consider tanning bed use a worthwhile activity despite potential hazards); and factor 3, danger ubiquity rationalizations (ie, beliefs normalizing the dangers of tanning bed use because of the ubiquity of risks).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Item Endorsement Labels and Scale Reliabilities

We examined the endorsement (percentage of current tanners who chose “agree” or “strongly agree” responses) of each item included in our cognitive rationalization scale (Table). We used a cutoff point of 10% and deleted the items that were not endorsed by at least 10% of the participants. Item endorsement clearly reflects the high endorsement of danger ubiquity rationalizations that normalize the dangers of tanning bed use because of the ubiquity of risks.

This exploratory study aimed to examine item response distribution of an adapted cognitive rationalization scale by tanning bed users. The results indicated that current tanners endorse danger ubiquity rationalizations most strongly, but other rationalizations are endorsed moderately, suggesting the need for more qualitative work to uncover other rationalizations.

Strong motivations for tanning bed use also include peer norms, parental norms, and other sociocultural influences to use tanning beds,6 but these motivations were not reflected in the adapted cognitive rationalization scale. More in-depth qualitative work may uncover other rationalizations that tanning bed users may offer when they are made aware that they continue to use tanning beds despite awareness of risks associated with usage.

The results presented here should be interpreted with caution given the small sample size, cross-sectional data, and the lack of demographic background data. We adapted a preexisting cognitive rationalization scale (on smoking behavior) to tanning bed use. Given that these 2 behaviors are very different in context, and endorsement is moderate, a more complete measure would need to include more strongly endorsed rationalizations, possibly through rich focus group data.

Correspondence: Dr Banerjee, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center, 641 Lexington Ave, Seventh Floor, New York, NY 10022 (banerjes@mskcc.org).

Accepted for Publication: January 30, 2012.

Author Contributions: All authors had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Banerjee, Hay, and Greene. Acquisition of data: Banerjee and Greene. Analysis and interpretation of data: Banerjee and Hay. Drafting of the manuscript: Banerjee. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Banerjee, Hay, and Greene. Statistical analysis: Banerjee. Administrative, technical, and material support: Banerjee and Greene. Study supervision: Banerjee, Hay, and Greene.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Demierre MF. Time for the national legislation of indoor tanning to protect minors.  Arch Dermatol. 2003;139(4):520-524
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lazovich D, Vogel RI, Berwick M, Weinstock MA, Anderson KE, Warshaw EM. Indoor tanning and risk of melanoma: a case-control study in a highly exposed population.  Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2010;19(6):1557-1568
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Stapleton J, Turrisi R, Hillhouse J, Robinson JK, Abar B. A comparison of the efficacy of an appearance-focused skin cancer intervention within indoor tanner subgroups identified by latent profile analysis.  J Behav Med. 2010;33(3):181-190
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Festinger L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row; 1957
Oakes W, Chapman S, Borland R, Balmford J, Trotter L. “Bulletproof skeptics in life's jungle”: which self-exempting beliefs about smoking most predict lack of progression towards quitting?  Prev Med. 2004;39(4):776-782
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cafri G, Thompson JK, Roehrig M,  et al.  Appearance motives to tan and not tan: evidence for validity and reliability of a new scale.  Ann Behav Med. 2008;35(2):209-220
PubMed   |  Link to Article

Figures

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Item Endorsement Labels and Scale Reliabilities

References

Demierre MF. Time for the national legislation of indoor tanning to protect minors.  Arch Dermatol. 2003;139(4):520-524
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lazovich D, Vogel RI, Berwick M, Weinstock MA, Anderson KE, Warshaw EM. Indoor tanning and risk of melanoma: a case-control study in a highly exposed population.  Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2010;19(6):1557-1568
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Stapleton J, Turrisi R, Hillhouse J, Robinson JK, Abar B. A comparison of the efficacy of an appearance-focused skin cancer intervention within indoor tanner subgroups identified by latent profile analysis.  J Behav Med. 2010;33(3):181-190
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Festinger L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row; 1957
Oakes W, Chapman S, Borland R, Balmford J, Trotter L. “Bulletproof skeptics in life's jungle”: which self-exempting beliefs about smoking most predict lack of progression towards quitting?  Prev Med. 2004;39(4):776-782
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cafri G, Thompson JK, Roehrig M,  et al.  Appearance motives to tan and not tan: evidence for validity and reliability of a new scale.  Ann Behav Med. 2008;35(2):209-220
PubMed   |  Link to Article

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles
Introduction to Photobiology. Dermatol Clin 2014;32(3):255-266.